0 Members and 1 Guest are viewing this topic. Read 8291 times.
I may be, in the first paragraph the claim is that spatial averaging is responsible for timbre, accurate or not. And that these repetitions from many angles elicit an impression (increase our sensitivity) of an actual live performance (resonances with a distinctive timbre) only if the amplitude and delay are correct. In pursuit of that distinctive timbre I want to know how and what room treatments will address my very specific requirement since this method deals directly with correcting reflections for the better. I have never used any room treatment intentionally and remain sceptical for the most part.
In the second paragraph Mr. Toole appears to be hinting that something magical may happen under some circumstances I don't quite understand so, I must have quoted it out of context since he appears to be an objectivist.
To answer your question, everyone, without exception, can distinguish live from reproduced sound there's absolutely nothing controversial about it. If what you say were true how would any audiophile's ambition be resolved if there were no way to know when he “got there”?
This is an interesting subject for sure. I have a ton of respect for Toole's work, but I also disagree with him on a few key issues. For me this issue is related to one of the areas in which we disagree -- the importance of treating early reflections and controlling how much they are heard.For me it comes down to this assertion from Toole: "These reflected “repetitions” of the direct sound have a second benefit, increasing our sensitivity to the subtle resonances that give sounds their distinctive timbres." If this assertion is the case (ie, true) then it follows that we would be less sensitive to subtle resonances that gives sound their distinctive timbres in rooms without early reflections, ie, listening outdoors or in a room with a well-designed reflection-free zone. And we know this is NOT the case.
Early reflections are known to create distortion, comb filtering, and psychoacoustic challenges. These are sonic artifacts not present in the original recording, so anything the room adds will be definition be less accurate, not more. And much of the comb filtering happens in the midrange, which as Toole notes, is where our ears are most sensitive. For me this is a step away from accuracy, not toward.
Some people like the sound of a lively room, some people prefer the sound of a quiet room. I'm definitely in the latter camp.
It is just to say that what gives an instrument its timbre in real life is more than its direct frequency response. A trumpet, for example, has a narrow sound dispersion pattern and focuses all its energy forward. What makes a trumpet sound like a trumpet in a room is the combination of direct and reflected energy in that room, and the reflected energy is weaker than direct energy, especially at higher frequencies. If you play the sound of a trumpet through omnidirectional speakers, it won't sound like an actual trumpet would sound in your room because the balance of direct and reflected energy is way off. Similarly, if you play the sound of something that is mostly omnidirectional, like a xylophone, through narrow directivity speakers like horns, it won't sound like an actual xylophone would sound in your room.