0 Members and 1 Guest are viewing this topic. Read 4918 times.
You mentioned high efficiency speakers. Many high efficiency speakers have fairly well-controlled radiation patterns, and in that regard at least they'd be a step in the right direction in my opinion. They also tend to have good liveliness even at low volume levels. Omega comes to mind - I used to be an Omega dealer, and think very highly of the line. Duke
Regarding the imaging being "killed" by reflection-induced frequency response differences at the two ears, I'm not so sure. The ear/brain system derives directional cues primarily from the first .68 milliseconds (corresponding to roughly 8 inches path length) of a sound impulse. After this, the precedence effect, or Haas effect (after Helum Haas) kicks in, suppressing directional cues from reflections of the original sound. That being said, a strong, distinct ("specular") early reflection can still skew the apparent direction of a sound source - which is why diffusion or (if necessary) absorption of first reflection energy is desirable. The Haas effect continues for 40 milliseconds or so (if I recall correctly), after which a reflection is distinctly heard as an echo. During this interval the ear/brain system is still picking up timbral and loudness cues, but that's a different topic.
Well I do see what you mean about the different frequency responses being created at each ear by the different reflection patterns, and I don't know of any study that specifically examines this factor.
I think the Haas effect would still suppress the directional cues because small dissimilarities between the direct and reverberant sound don't de-rail it; it still works unless the discrepancy is very large. This dissimilar-at-each-ear set of reflections would have been present in studies that used loudspeakers for sound sources, but not in studies that used headphones. They would also be present in live sounds, which "image" pretty well under most conditions (and not under some).
Regarding the ideal "balance" between absorption and diffusion, I think that to a certain extent personal preference enters in (imagine that!).
I tend to place a higher priority on getting rich timbre along with a sense of ambience or even envelopment, rather than on sound image localization.
Also, in my experience excess absorption can skew the timbre, as most absorptive materials are far more effective at short wavelengths than at longer ones.
What is your opinion on nearfield listening? It's an effective way to make sure the direct sound dominates the reverberant sound, and I wonder what you have found regarding nearfield listening in a "typical room" (whatever that is) versus farfield listening in an absorptively-treated room.
if ambient energy is undesirable in a small room, the implication is that narrow-pattern loudspeakers would be preferable.
I read your paper entitled "Why we believe", and, well, you might be right or at least partially right (I still think human hearing perception works very differently from the way we currently measure amplifier distortion, for example - so I give credence to those who prefer amps that "measure" poorly).
Blauert takes the position that spaciousness is a good thing, but if (big "if") spaciousness is the opposite of precise imaging, and if precise imaging is your priority, then yes it looks to me like comb filtering is indeed detrimental to such imaging. And a highly absorptive environment would certainly minimize comb filtering.
It's too bad we live so far apart, as I suspect you could teach me a lot in a few ears-on demonstrations. Thanks for sharing your work with us through your writings.
the ear is ... intolerant of very small amounts of higher-order distortion.
Negative feedback and amplifier crossover distortion are types of distortion the ear finds highly detectable and objectionable.
what I should have said is that one of the things negative feedback does is trade off high percentages of low-order harmonic distortion for low percentages of high-order harmonic distortion.
Earl commented to me that his study indicates a negative correlation between lower THD percentages and listener preference.
I have a second system in my home office which is a relatively small square room - 13 x 13. I have loaded up the room with my best gear but as much as I love my Ellis 1801s they are just too much for this small room. So they are going back to my large living room (18x30) where they will be much happier. So thinking about my small room the questions I have are:1. what qualities should I look for from a speaker for a small room like mine2. what is working for you in a small room2. speaker recommendations that will sound great but won't overwhelm a small roomRest of gear in the room is modded PS Audio amp, Audio Note preamp, modwright Sony cd player, and squeezebox.
Total harmonic distortion is lowered by negative feedback, but the distortion envelope is changed and high-order harmonic distortion is actually increased in level.
If 10% THD with a certain distortion envelope is rated as "imperceptible" by 37 blind listeners, and if .01% THD with another distortion envelope is rated as "intolerable" by those same listeners, then THD utterly fails as a distortion metric if our goal is correlation with human hearing (see Geddes and Lee, references below, where this is exactly what is shown).
it is the higher odd-order harmonics that are most objectionable.
I now think that the even-order harmonics are reduced as you have claimed (lower ones being reduced more than the higher ones), but not the odd-order harmonics.
Odd-order harmonics are rare and unnatural in nature
By way of introduction to the papers, and eventually coming back to the issue of how .01% THD can be not only audible but unacceptable, let me pull a few quotes from Geddes' book, "Audio Transducers":
"If the amplifier has crossover distortion then this type of nonlinearity violates both of our principles - it is both very high order and it increases (as a proportion of the linear terms) with decreasing signal level. One would expect, based on our hypothesis, that this type of distortion would be highly objectionable and it is"
"So basically our new "metric" is the actual parameters of the nonlinear components themselves, or the frequency response of the orders, weighted by their order and required to only grow with level (again relative to the linear term). It is not that uncommon to see discussions of the 2nd and 3rd order nonlinearity - we did it ourselves - but it is rare to see a discussion of the higher order nonlinearity. If increasing order are indeed more audible than lower orders then limiting our discussion to only the lower two orders is seriously flawed. The ROOT CAUSE of distortion is the underlying nonlinearity of the system or subsystem and the correct way to discuss nonlinearity is with the orders of its nonlinear transfer function. When one views the distortion problem in this way, signal based distortion metrics (IM, THD, etc.) become irrelevant." (Taken from pages 236 - 241 of "Audio Transducers" by Earl Geddes.)
Regarding your test of the audibility of a 3 kHz signal in the presence of a 100 Hz signal that's 80 dB louder, I think this is applicable to distortion perception but I am not sure how much so (the 3 kHz signal is another sine wave rather than a distortion, so it might be more tolerable than a distortion would be).
Note also that signal level would play a role - if the 100 Hz signal is at a level of 70 dB, and the 3 kHz signal is 80 dB down, then it's not surprising that the 3 kHz signal would be undetectable.
Finally, correct me if my math is wrong here, but I think that .01% would be four orders of magnitude down in level, or 40 dB down relative to the main signal, instead of 80 dB down.